top of page
Search
Norman Fenton

Paradoxes in the reporting of Covid19 vaccine effectiveness

The full pdf version of the following article (which includes the Appendix) can be found here.



Paradoxes in the reporting of Covid19 vaccine effectiveness

Why current studies (for or against vaccination) cannot be trusted and what we can do about it

Norman Fenton, Martin Neil and Scott McLachlan

Risk Information and Management Research

School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science,

Queen Mary University of London

15 Sept 2021

The randomized controlled trials (RCTs) to establish the safety and effectiveness of Covid19 vaccines produced impressive results (Polack et al., 2020) but were inevitably limited in the way they assessed safety (Folegatti et al., 2020)[1] and are effectively continuing (Ledford, Cyranoski, & Van Noorden, 2020; Singh et al., 2021) . Ultimately, the safety and effectiveness of these vaccines will be determined by real world observational data over the coming months and years.


However, data from observational studies on vaccine effectiveness can easily be misinterpreted leading to incorrect conclusions. For example, we previously noted[2] the Public Health England data shown in Figure 1 for Covid19 cases and deaths of vaccinated and unvaccinated people up to June 2021. Overall, the death rate was three times higher in the vaccinated group, leading many to conclude that vaccination increases the risk of death from Covid19. But this conclusion was wrong for this data because, in each of the different age categories (under 50 and 50+), the death rate was lower in the vaccinated group.




Figure 1 Data from Public Health England, June 2021


This is an example of Simpson’s paradox (Pearl & Mackenzie, 2018). It arises here because most vaccinated people were in the 50+ category where most deaths occur. Specifically: a) a much higher proportion of those aged 50+ were vaccinated compared to those aged <50; and b) those aged 50+ are much more likely to die.


So, as shown in Figure 2(a), ‘age’ is a confounding variable. While it is reasonable to assume that death is dependent on age, in a proper RCT to determine the effectiveness of the vaccine we would need to break the dependency of vaccination on age as shown in Figure 2(b), by ensuring the same proportion of people were vaccinated in each age category.






Figure 2 Causal model reflecting the observed data


The Appendix demonstrates how this causal model, and Bayesian inference, can both explain the paradox and avoid it (by simulating an RCT). Using the model in Figure 2 (b), which avoids the confounding effect of age, we conclude (based only on the data in this study) that the (relative) risk of death is four times higher in the unvaccinated (0.417%) than the vaccinated (0.104%), meaning the absolute increase in risk of death is 0.313% greater for the unvaccinated.


An excellent article by Jeffrey Morris[3] demonstrates the paradox in more detail using more recent data from Israel.


Clearly confounding factors like age (and also comorbidities) must, therefore, always be considered to avoid underestimating vaccine effectiveness data. However, the conclusions of these studies are also confounded by failing to consider non-Covid deaths, which will overestimate the safety of the vaccine if there were serious adverse reactions.

In fact, there are many other confounding factors that can compromise the results of any observational study into vaccine effectiveness (Krause et al., 2021). By ‘compromise’ we mean not just over- or under-estimate effectiveness, but - as in the example above - may completely reverse the results if we fail to adjust even for a single confounder (Fenton, Neil, & Constantinou, 2019).


In particular, the following usually ignored confounding factors will certainly overestimate vaccine effectiveness. These include:


  • The classification of Covid19 deaths and hospitalizations. For those classified as Covid19 cases who die (whether due to Covid19 or some other condition), there is the issue of whether the patient is classified as dying ‘with’ Covid19 or ‘from’ Covid19. There may be differences between vaccinated and unvaccinated in the way this classification is made. The same applies to patients classified as Covid19 cases who are hospitalized.

  • The number of doses and amount of time since last dose used to classify whether a person has been vaccinated. For example, any person testing positive for Covid19 or dying of any cause within 14 days of their second dose is now classified by the CDC as ‘unvaccinated’ (CDC, 2021). While this definition may make sense for determining effectiveness in preventing Covid19 infections, it may drastically overestimate vaccine safety; this is because most serious adverse reactions from vaccines in general occur in the first 14 days (Scheifele, Bjornson, & Johnston, 1990; Stone, Rukasin, Beachkofsky, Phillips, & Phillips, 2019) and the same applies to Covid19 vaccines (Farinazzo et al., 2021; Mclachlan et al., 2021). There is also growing evidence that people hospitalized for any reason within 14 days of a vaccination are classified as unvaccinated and, for many, as Covid19 cases[4].

  • The accuracy of Covid19 testing and Covid19 case classification. These are critical factors since there may be different testing strategies for the unvaccinated compared to the vaccinated. For example, in the large observation study of the Pfizer vaccine effectiveness in Israel (Haas et al., 2021) unvaccinated asymptomatic people were much more likely to be tested than vaccinated asymptomatic people, resulting in the unvaccinated being more likely to be classified as Covid19 cases than vaccinated[5].

Even if we wish to simply study the effectiveness of the vaccine with respect to avoiding Covid infection (as opposed to avoiding death or hospitalization) there are many more factors that need to be considered than currently are. To properly account for the interacting effects of all relevant factors that ultimately impact (or explain) observed data we need a causal model such as that in Figure 3.





Figure 3 Causal model to determine vaccine effectiveness


As in the simple model of Figure 2, the nodes in the model shown in Figure 3 correspond to relevant factors (some of which relate to individuals – like age, and some of which relate to the population – like whether lockdowns are in place) and an arc from one node to another means there is a direct causal/influential dependence in the direction of the arc. For example: younger people – and those who have immunity from previous Covid infection – are less likely to be vaccinated than older people; older people are more likely to have comorbidities and more likely to have symptoms if they are infected. However, while those factors and relationships are widely considered in observational studies, most of the other factors in the model are not.


The first thing to note is that the model makes clear the critical distinction between whether a person is Covid19 infected (something which is not easily observable) and whether they are classified as a Covid19 case (i.e. the ones who are recorded as cases in any given study). The latter depends not just on whether they are genuinely infected but also on the accuracy of the testing and whether they are vaccinated. If (as in the Israel study described above) the unvaccinated are subject to more extensive (and potentially inaccurate) testing, then they are more likely to be erroneously classified as a case. The model also makes clear the critical distinction between those who have been vaccinated (at least once) and those classified as vaccinated in the study. The latter depends on the number of doses, time since last dose, and whether the person tests positive. Moreover, whether a person gets more than one dose will depend on whether they suffered an adverse reaction first time; those who do and who do not get a second dose are generally classified as unvaccinated - and this will compromise any studies of risk associated with the vaccine. Indeed, even the results of randomized controlled trials were compromised both by ‘removing’ those who died within 14 days of the second vaccination and ‘losing’ many subjects after the first dose[6].


The causal model makes clear that a person cannot become infected with the virus unless they come into contact with it. The latter depends not just on age, ethnicity and profession (so young people who live, work and travel in crowded environments are more likely to come into contact with the virus as are any people in a hospital environment) but also on changing population factors like lockdown restrictions in place and current population infection rate. Assuming a person comes into contact with the virus, whether they get infected depends on whether they have natural immunity and whether they are vaccinated.


If we had relevant data on all of the factors in the model then, as in the case of the simple model in the Appendix, we can capture the probabilistic dependence between each node and its immediate parents, and then use Bayesian inference to determine the true effect of vaccination. In principle, this enables us to properly explain all observed data, adjust for all confounding factors, and provide truly accurate measures of effectiveness. The problem is that several key variables are generally unobservable directly while many of the easily observable variables are simply not recorded. While we can incorporate expert judgment with observed statistical data to populate the model, this can be extremely complex and subjective.


Moreover, if you think the model is already very complex, then it should be noted that it is far from fully comprehensive. Even before we consider all the additional factors and relationships needed to consider the outcomes of hospitalization and death (and the accuracy of reporting these), the model does not take account of: different treatments given; different morbidities and lifestyle choices; seasons over which data are collected; different strains of the virus; and many other factors. Nor does it account for the fact that all observational data are biased (or ‘censored’) in the sense that it only contains information on people who are available for the study; so, for example, studies in particular countries will largely contain people of a specific ethnicity, while all studies will generally exclude certain classes of people (such as the homeless). This means that, while such studies could be useful in determining effectiveness at a ‘local’ level, their conclusions are not generalizable. Indeed, they may are completely unreliable because of another paradox (called collider or Berkson’s paradox) unless we have explicitly adjusted for this as described in (Fenton, 2020).


Given the impossibility of controlling for all these factors in randomized trials, and the overwhelming complexity of adjusting for them from observational data there is little we can reliably conclude from the data and studies so far. And we have not even mentioned the general failure of these studies to consider the impact and trade-offs of safety on effectiveness.


So, what can we do about this mess? We believe there is an extremely simple and objective solution: if we ignore the cost of vaccination, then ultimately we can all surely agree that the vaccine is effective overall if there are fewer deaths (from any cause) among the vaccinated than the unvaccinated. This combines both effectiveness and safety since it encapsulates the trade-off between them. It is not perfect, because there could be systemic differences in treatments given to vaccinated and unvaccinated[7], but it completely bypasses the problem of classifying Covid19 ‘cases’ which, as we have noted, compromises all studies so far.


So, provided that we can agree on an objective way to classify a person as vaccinated (and we propose that, for this purpose, the fairest way is to define anybody as vaccinated if they have received at least one dose), then all we need to do is compare all-cause mortality rates in different age categories of the vaccinated v unvaccinated over a period of several months[8].


A recent analysis does indeed look at all-cause deaths in vaccinated and unvaccinated (Classen, 2021). The study shows that, for all three of the vaccines for which data were available, all-cause deaths is significantly higher in the vaccinated than the unvaccinated. However, this study did not account for age and hence its conclusions are also unreliable.


We could immediately evaluate the effectiveness to date of vaccines in the UK by simply looking at the registered deaths since the start of the vaccination programme in December 2020. All we need to know for each registered death is the person’s age and whether they received at least one dose of the vaccine before death. Although a longer period would, of course, be better it is still sufficiently long to show a real effect if the vaccines work as claimed and if Covid19 is as deadly as claimed.


Moving forward we should certainly be collecting this simple data, but our concern is that (in many countries) the ‘control group’ (i.e. unvaccinated) may soon not be large enough for such a simple evaluation.

References

CDC. (2021). COVID-19 Breakthrough Case Investigations and Reporting | CDC. Retrieved September 15, 2021, from https://www.cdc.gov/vaccines/covid-19/health-departments/breakthrough-cases.html

Classen, B. (2021). US COVID-19 Vaccines Proven to Cause More Harm than Good Based on Pivotal Clinical Trial Data Analyzed Using the Proper Scientific Endpoint, “All Cause Severe Morbidity.” Trends in Internal Medicine, 1(1), 1–6. Retrieved from https://www.scivisionpub.com/pdfs/us-covid19-vaccines-proven-to-cause-more-harm-than-good-based-on-pivotal-clinical-trial-data-analyzed-using-the-proper-scientific--1811.pdf

Farinazzo, E., Ponis, G., Zelin, E., Errichetti, E., Stinco, G., Pinzani, C., … Zalaudek, I. (2021). Cutaneous adverse reactions after m‐RNA COVID‐19 vaccine: early reports from Northeast Italy. Journal of the European Academy of Dermatology and Venereology, 35(9), e548–e551. https://doi.org/10.1111/jdv.17343

Fenton, N. (2020). Why most studies into COVID19 risk factors may be producing flawed conclusions - and how to fix the problem. ArXiv. https://doi.org/http://arxiv.org/abs/2005.08608

Fenton, N. E., Neil, M., & Constantinou, A. (2019). Simpson’s Paradox and the implications for medical trials. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1912.01422

Folegatti, P. M., Ewer, K. J., Aley, P. K., Angus, B., Becker, S., Belij-Rammerstorfer, S., … Oxford COVID Vaccine Trial Group. (2020). Safety and immunogenicity of the ChAdOx1 nCoV-19 vaccine against SARS-CoV-2: a preliminary report of a phase 1/2, single-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet (London, England), 396(10249), 467–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)31604-4

Haas, E. J., Angulo, F. J., McLaughlin, J. M., Anis, E., Singer, S. R., Khan, F., … Alroy-Preis, S. (2021). Impact and effectiveness of mRNA BNT162b2 vaccine against SARS-CoV-2 infections and COVID-19 cases, hospitalisations, and deaths following a nationwide vaccination campaign in Israel: an observational study using national surveillance data. Lancet (London, England), 397(10287), 1819–1829. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00947-8

Krause, P. R., Fleming, T. R., Peto, R., Longini, I. M., Figueroa, J. P., Sterne, J. A. C., … Henao-Restrepo, A.-M. (2021). Considerations in boosting COVID-19 vaccine immune responses. The Lancet, 0(0). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(21)02046-8

Ledford, H., Cyranoski, D., & Van Noorden, R. (2020). The UK has approved a COVID vaccine — here’swhat scientists now want to know. Retrieved from https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-020-03441-8?utm_source=Nature+Briefing&utm_campaign=597ee8dba8-briefing-dy-20201203&utm_medium

Mclachlan, S., Osman, M., Dube, K., Chiketero, P., Choi, Y., & Fenton, N. (2021). Analysis of COVID-19 vaccine death reports from the Vaccine Adverse Events Reporting System (VAERS) Database Interim: Results and Analysis. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.26987.26402

Pearl, J., & Mackenzie, D. (2018). The book of why : the new science of cause and effect. New York: Basic Books.

Polack, F. P., Thomas, S. J., Kitchin, N., Absalon, J., Gurtman, A., Lockhart, S., … C4591001 Clinical Trial Group. (2020). Safety and Efficacy of the BNT162b2 mRNA Covid-19 Vaccine. The New England Journal of Medicine, 383(27), 2603–2615. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa2034577

Scheifele, D. W., Bjornson, G., & Johnston, J. (1990). Evaluation of adverse events after influenza vaccination in hospital personnel. CMAJ : Canadian Medical Association Journal = Journal de l’Association Medicale Canadienne, 142(2), 127–130. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2295029

Singh, J. A., Kochhar, S., Wolff, J., Atuire, C., Bhan, A., Emanuel, E., … Upshur, R. E. G. (2021). Placebo use and unblinding in COVID-19 vaccine trials: recommendations of a WHO Expert Working Group. Nature Medicine, 27(4), 569–570. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-021-01299-5

Stone, C. A., Rukasin, C. R. F., Beachkofsky, T. M., Phillips, E. J., & Phillips, E. J. (2019). Immune‐mediated adverse reactions to vaccines. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 85(12), 2694–2706. https://doi.org/10.1111/bcp.14112


[1] Some participants and sites were unblinded and non-randomised and others were effectively unblinded when they received paracetamol prior to jab [2] https://probabilityandlaw.blogspot.com/2021/06/simpsons-paradox-in-interepretation.html [3] https://www.covid-datascience.com/post/israeli-data-how-can-efficacy-vs-severe-disease-be-strong-when-60-of-hospitalized-are-vaccinated [4] https://www.bitchute.com/video/lXrcpFe4V4U2/ [5] https://probabilityandlaw.blogspot.com/2021/05/important-caveats-to-pfizer-vaccine.html [6] Some of the covid vax trials were unblinded, others were only single-blinded. Yet more were non-randomised and others were accidentally unblinded when the treatment recipients were given paracetamol prior to their covid jab [7] There are multiple anecdotal reports that Australian hospitals are now giving ivermectin only to vaccinated patients [8] https://probabilityandlaw.blogspot.com/2021/06/why-all-studies-so-far-into-risks-andor.html

14,004 views5 comments

Recent Posts

See All

5 Comments


noakesr8
noakesr8
Nov 22, 2021

Covid Crusher: Mix one heaped teaspoon of Iodine table or sea salt in a mug of warm clean water, cup a hand and sniff or snort the entire mugful up your nose, spitting out anything which comes down into your mouth. If sore, then you have a virus, so continue morning noon and night, or more often if you want, until the soreness goes away (2-3 minutes) then blow out your nose and flush away, washing your hands afterwards, until when you do my simple cure, you don't have any soreness at all, when you flush - job done. Also swallow a couple of mouthfuls of salt water and if you have burning in your lungs, salt killing virus and…

Like

tchebycheff
Sep 25, 2021

Reliable comparison of small probabilities requires large sample sizes. For example, if we take two groups of 50,000 size each and identical IFR of 0.05% and simulate RRR, we can see RRR's all over the place (std dev 30% over 100 trials!).


It is near useless to compare IFR of the order seen with the sample sizes of the order listed above.

Like

soloman
soloman
Sep 16, 2021

This is a problem with the history of vaccination. No debate is allowed anymore and anybody who points out that proper controls are needed is labelled stupid undeducated anti=vax terrorist blah blah . HOwever you cannot prove the safety or effectiveness of any vaccine over time without following up controls. So the entire history of so called wonderful vaccines is floored. Who follows the controls? Well, some people do and in USA a white paper has showed that unvaccinated children are healthier than vaccinated. My belief is that we are over vaccinating children now and this is causing problems. It is well known now that the overuse of anti biotics is rendering them in many cases no longer effective, y…

Like

pijus magnificus
pijus magnificus
Sep 15, 2021

I think that in assessing the effect of the vaccine, there is a critical factor that you have not taken into account that affects the virus (and therefore vaccinated and unvaccinated alike): the vaccine exerts selective pressure on the virus and causes vaccine-resistant marginal variants to become predominant.

This implies that we cannot evaluate the vaccine by simply comparing vaccinated and unvaccinated.

For example, when vaccination began in the UK the Alpha variant became widespread. Alpha is ~50% more contagious than the original European variants.

Delta spread with the start of vaccination in India. Delta is more than 100% more contagious than the original European variants.

The viral loads of those infected with Delta are much higher than the viral…


Like
jimpad11
Sep 16, 2021
Replying to

"there is a critical factor that you have not taken into account that affects the virus .... the vaccine exerts selective pressure on the virus and causes vaccine-resistant marginal variants to become predominant"


I agree. I'm a retired Scientist who spent my career as a Sr executive because of an innate ability to observe large amounts of real world data and notice correlation between apparently unrelated data from different situations

I mention this only because I noticed that the available data seemed to suggest a possible selective pressure exerted by the highly specific immunity (antibodies specific to the S-protein antigen) induced by the vaccines. Natural (infection acquired) immunity is less specific and recent studies have shown it to be at…


Like
bottom of page